Dr Dagmar Zeithamova | How Our Brains Create Categories: A Look Inside the Mind
Our brains have an incredible ability to organise information into meaningful categories, allowing us to make sense of the world around us. However, this categorisation process can also bias how we perceive and remember this world. Dr Dagmar Zeithamova and her team at the University of Oregon are uncovering how our brains form category-biased representations, even when we’re focused on learning about individual examples. Their work sheds new light on the neural mechanisms underlying concept formation and how categories shape our perceptions.
The Power of Categories
From the moment we wake up, our brains are constantly sorting the flood of sensory information we encounter into meaningful categories. This ability to organise our experiences allows us to navigate the world efficiently, make quick decisions, and draw on past knowledge to interpret new situations. However, our inclination to categorise can also lead to biases in how we perceive and remember individual items.
Previous research has shown that after learning to sort items into categories, we tend to perceive examples from the same category as more similar to each other than they actually are. At the same time, we perceive more significant differences between items from different categories. This ‘perceptual magnet effect’ can occur even for arbitrary categories created in laboratory settings.
While this categorical perception can be helpful in many situations, allowing us to focus on relevant similarities and differences, it can also lead us to overlook individual variations within a category. This tendency toward categorical thinking may contribute to harmful social biases like stereotyping. Understanding the cognitive and neural mechanisms behind category formation could provide insights into how such biases develop and potentially how to counteract them.
But how and when do these category-biased perceptions form in the brain? Do they only arise when we are explicitly trying to learn about categories? Or can simply learning category labels lead to spontaneous changes in how our brains represent individual examples? These are some of the key questions Dr Dagmar Zeithamova and her colleagues at the University of Oregon set out to investigate.
Unveiling Category Bias in the Brain
Dr Zeithamova, an associate professor of psychology, has long been fascinated by how the brain integrates information across experiences to form new knowledge. Her work combines behavioural experiments with advanced brain imaging techniques to peer into the neural mechanisms of learning and memory.
In a recent study, Dr Zeithamova and her team used an innovative experimental design to track how category learning shapes neural representations. They wanted to see if category-biased neural patterns would form even when people were focused on learning about individuating details rather than categories. The researchers created a set of face stimuli by blending photographs of different individuals. Some faces shared visual features because they were created by blending the same ‘parent’ faces. Crucially, some pairs of visually similar faces were assigned to the same category by giving them the same family name, while other similar pairs were assigned to different categories with different family names. This allowed the team to separate the effects of visual similarity from learned category membership.
Participants in the study were asked to learn the full names (both first and family names) for each face. Importantly, they were not told to focus on the categories or family names – their task was simply to memorise the individual name-face pairs. Grouping the faces in family categories was not really helpful for the task at hand as participants needed to differentiate ‘Peter Miller’ from ‘George Miller’ and ‘John Miller’. This meant any effects of categorisation would occur spontaneously rather than through explicit category learning.
As the participants studied the face-name pairs, their brain activity was recorded using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). This allowed the researchers to examine the patterns of neural activity associated with viewing each individual face. After learning, participants also completed perceptual similarity ratings and a surprise categorisation test with new face examples.
Tracking Category Information in the Brain
To analyse the fMRI data, Dr Zeithamova’s team used advanced pattern analysis techniques that can detect subtle differences in how information is represented across populations of neurons. They looked for evidence that the brain was encoding category-relevant information about the faces, even though participants weren’t explicitly trying to learn categories.
The results were striking. Even during initial learning, when participants were focused on memorising individual face-name pairs, their brains were spontaneously extracting and representing category information. The researchers could reliably decode which category (family name) a face belonged to based on the pattern of brain activity it evoked, particularly in regions involved in concept representation and generalisation like the middle temporal gyrus and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dr Zeithamova notes that this spontaneous formation of category-biased neural representations may help explain why we so readily generalise our experiences. Our brains seem primed to extract category-relevant patterns from our experiences, allowing us to apply knowledge to new situations. However, it may also contribute to over-generalisation and stereotyping in some cases.
From Neural Patterns to Behaviour
After the learning phase, participants were asked to rate the visual similarity of different face pairs. They also completed a surprise categorisation test where they had to assign new face examples to the correct family name categories. Interestingly, while the group as a whole did not show a significant behavioural category bias in their similarity ratings, there were individual differences. Participants who showed a stronger neural category bias were more likely to rate faces from the same category as more similar. They also performed better on the surprise categorisation test with new face examples.
This suggests that the strength of category representations formed during learning, even when not explicitly focused on categories, can influence later perception and the ability to generalise category knowledge to new examples. Dr Zeithamova points out that the neural measures may be more sensitive early indicators of categorical thinking compared to explicit behavioural measures.
Widespread Category Effects in the Brain
One surprising aspect of the results was how widespread the category effects were in the brain. While the researchers had predicted category-biased representations would form in regions previously implicated in concept learning and memory generalisation, like the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, they found category information distributed across many areas of the cortex. Even early visual processing regions showed some evidence of emphasising category-relevant visual features. This aligns with other research suggesting that conceptual knowledge can influence visual processing through top-down attentional effects.
Dr Zeithamova speculates that the widespread nature of the category representations may reflect attentional biasing toward category-relevant features across multiple levels of processing. Rather than being confined to high-level conceptual regions, category learning may alter how information is processed and represented throughout much of the cortex.
Practical Applications and Societal Impact
This research provides important insights into how our brains spontaneously organise information into meaningful categories, even when we’re not explicitly trying to learn or create categories. It demonstrates that simply being exposed to category labels can bias how our brains represent individual items, highlighting features relevant to category membership.
These findings have potential implications for understanding how stereotypes and social biases might form. Even without intentionally trying to create categories, our brains may extract and emphasise similarities among individuals assigned to the same group. This could contribute to overlooking individual differences and overgeneralising based on group membership.
Dr Zeithamova emphasises that categories themselves are not inherently problematic – they are a crucial tool for efficiently organising knowledge. The key is being aware of when categorical thinking may lead us astray and developing strategies to look beyond category labels when needed. As we navigate an increasingly complex world, understanding the cognitive mechanisms behind categorisation and generalisation is more crucial than ever. Dr Zeithamova and her colleagues are shedding new light on how our brains make sense of the world around us, one category at a time.
SHARE
DOWNLOAD E-BOOK
REFERENCE
https://doi.org/10.33548/SCIENTIA1157
MEET THE RESEARCHER
Dr Dagmar (Dasa) Zeithamova Demircan
Department of Psychology
University of Oregon
Eugene, OR
USA
Dr Dagmar (Dasa) Zeithamova Demircan is an associate professor at the Department of Psychology and Institute of Neuroscience at the University of Oregon. Her research focuses on the formation of knowledge and flexible use of experience, with particular emphasis on mechanisms supporting memory specificity and generalisation. Dr Zeithamova employs a combination of functional MRI, computational modelling, and behavioural methods to investigate how the brain builds conceptual knowledge from individual experiences. She has made significant contributions to understanding the roles of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in category learning and memory integration. Dr Zeithamova’s work has been supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health, and she has received recognition for her research and mentorship, including the 2024 Faculty Research Mentor Award from the University of Oregon and a Humboldt Research Fellowship for Experienced Researchers in 2022.
CONTACT
E: dasa@uoregon.edu
FURTHER READING
CR Bowman, T Iwashita, D Zeithamova, Tracking prototype and exemplar representations in the brain across learning, eLife, 2020, 9, e59360. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59360
L Frank, CR Bowman, D Zeithamova, Differential functional connectivity along the long axis of the hippocampus aligns with differential role in memory specificity and generalization, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2019, 31(12), 1958–1975. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01457
D Zeithamova, ML Mack, K Braunlich, et al., Brain mechanisms of concept learning, Journal of Neuroscience, 2019, 39(42), 8259–8266. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1166-19.2019
REPUBLISH OUR ARTICLES
We encourage all formats of sharing and republishing of our articles. Whether you want to host on your website, publication or blog, we welcome this. Find out more
Creative Commons Licence (CC BY 4.0)
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
What does this mean?
Share: You can copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format
Adapt: You can change, and build upon the material for any purpose, even commercially.
Credit: You must give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made.
SUBSCRIBE NOW
Follow Us
MORE ARTICLES YOU MAY LIKE
Dr Benjamin Bradley | Cause, Consequence, and Natural Selection: A New Vision of Darwin’s Psychological Work
The theory of evolution by natural selection stands at the heart of modern biology. But what exactly is the causal status of natural selection in evolution? Dr Benjamin Bradley from Charles Sturt University in Australia is challenging long-held assumptions, arguing that Darwin himself saw natural selection as a consequence of other processes, not a cause of evolution in its own right. This crucial distinction opens up new perspectives on how evolution relates to psychology and behaviour.
Professor Steven Wayne Evans | Making a Difference for Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
Tailored interventions are required if adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) are to meet their full potential. Research by Professor Steven Evans at Ohio University illuminates the effectiveness of these interventions and offers valuable insights for families, educators, and clinicians. From the significant impact of compliance with interventions to the diverse response patterns among adolescents, Professor Evans’s research underscores the importance of personalised approaches in fostering success. By understanding the nuances of ADHD management and investing in evidence-based strategies, clinicians and educators can better support the well-being and future prospects of adolescents navigating this complex neurodevelopmental condition.
Dr Robert W Stackman, Jr – Dr Sarah J Cohen | Of Mice and Matching: When Rodents Show They Can Match Pictures to Objects
Cancer research is an ever-evolving field of science fuelled by the pressure of the ongoing need to find new approaches to tackle this killer. Dr Zhi-yong Huang is based at the Tongji Medical College at Huazhong University of Science and Technology in China. He works tirelessly to develop novel treatments for diseases of the liver and biliary system, with a particular focus on liver cancer surgery. He has pioneered methods to optimise the treatment of this type of cancer.
Leadership – How Does Successful Leadership Work in Times of Change?
The digital revolution has changed our world – ‘VUCA’ – the conditions which now shape the lives of many people worldwide. VUCA stands for volatility, uncertainty, complexity, and ambiguity. In order to competently master challenges related to work structures and health, the increase in mental illnesses, the shortage of skilled workers or taking responsibility for one’s health, healthy leadership is considered a key factor.